H εκτίμηση της φωνολογικής βραχύχρονης μνήμης σε Ελληνόφωνα παιδιά 2 και 3 ετών : στοιχεία για τη σχέση της με την ανάπτυξη της γλώσσας

Part of : Προσχολική & σχολική εκπαίδευση ; Vol.3, No.2, 2015, pages 117-135

Issue:
Pages:
117-135
Author:
Abstract:
Η φωνολογική εργαζόμενη μνήμη (ΦΕΜ), όπως εκτιμάται μέσω της δοκιμασίας επανάληψης ψευδολέξεων, έχει συσχετισθεί με ποικίλες διαστάσεις της γλωσσικής ανάπτυξης των παιδιών. Παρότι οι περισσότερες έρευνες επικεντρώνονται σε ηλικίες μεγαλύτερες των 4 ετών, δεν απουσιάζουν από την διεθνή βιβλιογραφία επιτυχημένες προσπάθειες μέτρησης της ΦΕΜ σε μικρότερα παιδιά. Στόχος της παρούσας μελέτης ήταν, αφενός, η ανάπτυξη μιας δοκιμασίας που να καθιστά εφικτή την εκτίμηση της ΦΕΜ σε ελληνόφωνα παιδιά μικρής ηλικίας, και αφετέρου, μία αρχική διερεύνηση της σχέσης μεταξύ της επίδοσης στην δοκιμασία αυτή και του γλωσσικού επιπέδου των παιδιών. Για τις ανάγκες της έρευνας, 42 παιδιά ηλικίας 2 και 3 ετών συμμετείχαν σε δοκιμασία επανάληψης λέξεων και ψευδολέξεων η οποία δομήθηκε ειδικά για τις ηλικίες αυτές. Παράλληλα, συλλέχθηκε ένα δείγμα της ομιλίας των παιδιών βάσει του οποίου υπολογίστηκε ο ‘αριθμός διαφορετικών λέξεων’ (‘number of different words’) και το ‘μέσο μήκος εκφωνήματος σε μορφήματα’ (MLU-m). Τα αποτελέσματα καταδεικνύουν την ευαισθησία της δοκιμασίας στην ηλικία των παιδιών, στον τύπο του ερεθίσματος (λέξη-ψευδολέξη) και στον αριθμό συλλαβών. Επιπλέον, η επίδοση στην δοκιμασία παρουσιάζει σημαντικές συσχετίσεις με τους δείκτες γλωσσικής έκφρασης των παιδιών. Τα αποτελέσματα συζητούνται σε σχέση με προγενέστερα ευρήματα και τις δυνατότητες χρήσης της δοκιμασίας για την εκτίμηση του γλωσσικού επιπέδου ελληνόφωνων παιδιών μικρής ηλικίας.
Subject:
Subject (LC):
Keywords:
Φωνολογική Βραχύχρονη Μνήμη, Γλωσσική ανά̟πτυξη, Ε̟πανάληψη ψευδολέξεων, Εκφραστικό λεξιλόγιο, Μορφοσυντακτική ανά̟πτυξη, Μέσο Μήκος Εκφωνήματος, Phonological short-term memory, Language development, Non-word repetition, Productive vocabulary, Morphosyntactic development, Mean Length of Utterance (MLU)
References (1):
  1. Adams, A. (1996). Phonological working memory and spoken language development in young children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 49, 216-233. doi: 10.1080/713755610Adams, A., Bourke, L., & Willis, C. (1999). Working memory and spoken language comprehension in young children. International Journal of Psychology, 34, 364-373. doi: 10.1080/002075999399701Adams, A., & Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Phonological working memory and speech production in preschool children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 38, 403-414. doi:10.1044/jshr.3802.403Adams, A., & Gathercole, S. (2000). Limitation of working memory: implications for language development. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 35, 95-116. doi:10.1080/136828200247278Atkins, C. P., & Cartwright, L. R. (1982). An investigation of three language elicitation procedures on Head Start children. Language Speech Hearing Services in the Schools, 13, 33-36. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461.1301.33Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York, NY: Academic Press.Baddeley, A., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on shortterm storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586-595. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(88)90028-9Bailey, T. M., & Hahn, U. (2001). Determinants of wordlikeness: Phonotactics or lexical neighborhoods. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 568-591. doi:10.1006/jmla.2000.2756Bishop, D. V. M., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1996). Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 391–403. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01420.xBotting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2001). Nonword repetition and language development in children with language impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36, 421–432. doi: 10.1080/13682820110074971Burt, L., Holm, A., & Dodd, B. (1999). Phonological awareness skills of 4-year-old British children: An assessment and developmental data. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 34, 311–335. doi: 10.1080/136828299247432Campbell, T., Dollaghan, C., Needleman, H., & Janosky, J. (1997). Reducing bias in language assessment: Processing dependent measures. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 519-526. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4003.519Cheung, H. (1996). Nonword span as a unique predictor of second-language vocabulary learning. Developmental Psychology, 32, 867–873. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.32.5.867Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2007). The preschool repetition test: an evaluation of the performance in typically developing and clinically referred children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 507, 429-443. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/030)Chrysochoou, E., & Bablekou, Z. (2011). Phonological loop and central executive contributions to oral comprehension skills of 5.5 to 7.5 years old children. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 576-583. doi: 10.1002/acp.1723Chrysochoou, E., Bablekou, Z., Masoura, E., & Tsigilis, N. (2013). Working memory and vocabulary development in Greek preschool and primary school children. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 10, 417-432. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2012.686656Coady, J. A., & Aslin, R.N. (2004). Young children's sensitivity to probabilistic phonotactics in the developing lexicon. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89, 183-213. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.07.004Coady, J. A., & Evans, J. L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 43, 1-40. doi: 10.1080/13682820601116485Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for SLI. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 741-748. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00770de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (1999). Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 450–476. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.450Dollaghan, C., & Campbell, T. F. (1998). Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 1136–1146. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4105.1136Dufva, M., & Voeten, M. J. M. (1999). Native language literacy and phonological memory as prerequisites for learning English as a foreign language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 329–348. doi: 10.1017/S014271649900301XEdwards, J., Beckman, M. E., & Munson, B. (2004). The interaction between vocabulary size and phonotactic probability effects on children's production accuracy and fluency in nonword repetition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 421-436. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/034)Ellis, N. C., & Beaton, A. (1993). Factors affecting the learning of foreign language vocabulary: Imagery keyword mediators and phonological short-term memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 533-558. doi: 10.1080/14640749308401062Ellis Weismer, S., Tomblin, J. B., Zhang, X., Buckwalter, P., Chynoweth, J. G., & Jones, M. (2000). Nonword repetition performance in school-age children with and without language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 865–868. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4304.865Engel, P. M. J., Santos, F. H., & Gathercole, S. E. (2008). Are working memory measures free of socioeconomic influence? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 1580-1587. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0210)Frisch, S. A., Large, N. R., & Pisoni, D. B. (2000). Perception of word-likeness: effects of segment probability and length on processing non-words. Journal of Memory and Language, 42, 481-496. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1999.2692Garlock, V. M., Walley, A. C., & Metsala, J. L. (2001). Age-of-acquisition, word frequency and neighborhood density effects on spoken word recognition: Implications for the development of phoneme awareness and early reading ability. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 468-492. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2000.2784Gathercole, S. E. (1995). The assessment of phonological memory skills in preschool children. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 65, 155-164. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1995.tb01139.xGathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513-543. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.t01-1-00301.xGathercole, S. E., & Adams, A. M. (1993). Phonological working memory in very young children. Developmental Psychology, 29, 770–778. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.29.4.770Gathercole, S. E., & Adams, A. M. (1994). Children's phonological working memory: Contributions of long-term knowledge and rehearsal. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 672-688. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1994.1032Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. (1989). Evaluation of the role of STM in the development of vocabulary in children. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 336-360. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(89)90044-2Gathercole, S. E. & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and language. Hove, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd.Gathercole, S. E., Frankish, C., Pickering, S. J., & Peaker, S. (1999). Phonotactic influences on shortterm memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 84–95. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.25.1.84Gathercole, S. E., Hitch, G. J., Service, E., & Martin, A. J. (1997). Phonological short-term memory and new word learning in children. Developmental Psychology, 33, 966-979. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.966Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Baddeley, A. D., & Emslie, H. (1994). The Children's Test of Nonword Repetition: a test of phonological working memory. Memory, 2, 103-127. doi: 10.1080/09658219408258940Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1991). The influences of number of syllables and wordlikeness on children’s repetition of nonwords. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 349-367. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400009267Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28, 887-898. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.5.887Griffiths, Y. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2002). Predictors of exception word and nonword reading in dyslexic children: The severity hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 34-43. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.94.1.34Gupta, P., MacWhinney, B., Feldman, H. M., & Sacco, K. (2003). Phonological memory and vocabulary learning in children with focal lesions. Brain and Language, 87, 241-252. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00094-4Hoff, E., Core, C., & Bridges, K. (2008). Non-word repetition assesses phonological memory and is related to vocabulary development in 20- to 24-month-olds. Journal of Child Language, 35, 903-916. doi: 10.1017/S0305000908008751Hulme, C., & Mackenzie, S. (1992). Working memory and severe learning difficulties. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., & Hewes, A. K. (1999). Genetically dissociated components of working memory: Evidence from Down's and Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 37, 637-651. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00128-6Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., & Hewes, A. K. (2000). Verbal short-term memory deficits in Down syndrome: A consequence of problems in rehearsal? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 233-244. doi: 10.1017/S0021963099005120Καρούσου, A., & Πετρογιάννης, K. (2014). T.E.A. Κλίμακες για την ̟πρώιμη εκτίμηση της ε̟πικοινωνιακής και γλωσσικής ανά̟πτυξης ̟παιδιών ηλικίας 8-30 μηνών: Πιλοτικά δεδομένα και βασικές ψυχομετρικές ιδιότητες. Προσχολική και Σχολική Εκ̟αίδευση, 2, 83-114. doi: dx.doi.org/10.12681/ppej.47Karousou, A., & Petrogiannis, K. (2015). Communication Development Report [CDR] - A parentreport instrument for the early screening of communication and language development in Greek-speaking infants and toddlers. Manuscript submitted for publication.Kασσωτάκη-Μαριδάκη, Α. (1998). Ικανότητα βραχύχρονης συγκράτησης ̟πληροφοριών και ε̟πίδοση στην ανάγνωση: μια ̟προσ̟πάθεια διερεύνησης της σχέσης. Ψυχολογία, 5, 44-52.Κατή, Δ. (1992). Γλώσσα και ε̟ικοινωνία στο ̟παιδί. Αθήνα: Οδυσσέας.Landa, R. M., & Olswang, L. B. (1988). Effectiveness of language elicitation tasks with twoyear-olds. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 4, 170-192. doi: 10.1177/026565908800400204Laws, G. (1998). The use of nonword repetition as a test of phonological memory in children with Down syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 1119-1130. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00416Leadholm, B. J., & Miller, J. F (1992). Language sample analysis: The Wisconsin Guide. Madison, Wisconsin: Wisconsin Department of Public Instruction. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No ED371528). Ανακτήθηκε στις 15 Ιανουαρίου 2015 α̟πό http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED371528.pdfMackenzie, S., & Hulme, C. (1987). Memory span development in Down's syndrome, severely subnormal and normal subjects. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 4, 303-319. doi: 10.1080/02643298708252041Mariscal, S., & Gallego, C. (2013). La imitacion como herramienta para investigar y evaluar el desarrollo linguistico temprano: un estudio piloto de repeticion de palabras y pseudopalabras. Revista de investigacion en Logopedia, 3, 53-75.Masoura, E., & Gathercole, S.E. (1999). Phonological short-term memory and foreign vocabulary learning. International Journal of Psychology, 34, 383-388. doi: 10.1080/002075999399738Masoura, E., & Gathercole, S. E. (2005). Phonological short-term memory skills and new word learning in young Greek children. Memory, 13, 422-429.Μασούρα, Ε., Gathercole, S. E., & Μ̟παμ̟λέκου, Z. (2004). Η συμβολή της φωνολογικής βραχύχρονης μνήμης στην α̟πόκτηση λεξιλογίου. Ψυχολογία, 11, 341-355.Μασούρα Ε., Gathercole, S. E., & Μ̟παμ̟λέκου Ζ. (2006). Η εμ̟πλοκή της εργαζόμενης μνήμης σε έργα εκμάθησης νέων λέξεων: διερεύνηση σε μικρά ̟παιδιά. Στο Φ. Βλάχος, Φ. Μ̟ονώτη, Π. Μεταλλίδου, Ε. Δερμιτζάκη & Α. Ευκλείδη (Ε̟ιμ. Έκδ.), Ε̟πιστημονική Ε̟πετηρίδα της Ψυχολογικής Εταιρείας Βορείου Ελλάδας (Τόμος 4, σελ. 43 - 65). Αθήνα: Ελληνικά Γράμματα.Metsala, J. L. (2011). Lexical reorganization and the emergence of phonological awareness. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickenson, (Eds.), Handbook of Early Literacy Research (Vol. 3, pp. 66-82). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Miller, J. F. (1981). Assessing language production in children: Experimental procedures. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.Miyake, A., & Shah, P. (Eds.) (1999). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. New York: Cambridge University Press.Mueller, S. T, Seymour, T. L., Kieras, D. E., & Meyer, D. E. (2003). Theoretical implications of articulatory duration, phonological similarity, and phonological complexity in verbal working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 1353-1380. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.6.1353Munson, B. (2001). Phonological pattern frequency and speech production in adults and children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 778–792. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/061)Munson, B., Kurtz, B. A., & Windsor, J. (2005). The influence of vocabulary size, phonotactic probability, and wordlikeness on nonword repetitions of children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1033–1047. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/072)Muter, V., & Snowling, M. (1998). Concurrent and longitudinal predictors of reading: The role of metalinguistic and short-term memory skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 33, 320-337. doi: 10.1598/RRQ.33.3.4Oetting, J., & Cleveland, L. (2006). The utility of nonword repetition for southern African American and White children. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 20, 553-561. doi: 10.1080/02699200500266455Roy, P., & Chiat, S. (2004). A prosodically controlled word and nonword repetition task for 2- to 4-year-olds: Evidence from typically developing children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 223-34. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/019)Saffran, E. M. (1990). Short-term memory impairment and language processing. In A. Caramazza (Ed.), Cognitive neuropsychology and neurolinguistics. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Service, E., & Kohonen, V. (1995). Is the relation between phonological memory and foreign language learning accounted for by vocabulary acquisition? Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 155-172. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400007062Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511526817Snowling, M. (1981). Phonemic deficits in developmental dyslexia. Psychological Research, 43, 219-234. doi: 10.1007/BF00309831Storkel, H. L., Armbruster, J., & Hogan, T. P. (2006). Differentiating phonotactic probability and neighborhood density in adult word learning. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 1175-1192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/085)Thorn, A. S. C., & Gathercole, S. E. (1999). Language-specific knowledge and short-term memory in bilingual and non-bilingual children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52, 303-324. doi: 10.1080/713755823Wang, P. P., & Bellugi, U. (1994). Evidence from two genetic syndromes for a dissociation between verbal and visual-spatial short-term memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 317-322. doi: 10.1080/01688639408402641Willis, C. S. (1997). Working memory and children's language processing. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Lancaster: Lancaster University, UK.Zamuner, T. S., Gerken, L. A., & Hammond, M. (2004). Phonotactic probabilities in young children's speech production. Journal of Child Language, 31, 515-536. doi: 10.1017/S0305000904006233